Universal grammar

Universal grammar

Universal grammar is a theory of linguistics postulating principles of grammar shared by all languages, thought to be innate to humans (linguistic nativism). It attempts to explain language acquisition in general, not describe specific languages. Universal grammar proposes a set of rules intended to explain language acquisition in child development. The application of the idea to the area of second language acquisition (SLA) is represented mainly by the McGill University linguist Lydia White.

Some students of universal grammar study a variety of grammars to abstract generalizations called linguistic universals, often in the form of "If X holds true, then Y occurs." These have been extended to a range of traits, from the phonemes found in languages, to what word orders languages choose, to why children exhibit certain linguistic behaviors.as they considered issues of the Argument from poverty of the stimulus to arise from the constructivist approach to linguistic theory. The contrasting school of thought is known as functionalism.

History

The idea can be traced to Roger Bacon's observation that all languages are built upon a common grammar, substantially the same in all languages, even though it may undergo accidental variations, and the 13th century speculative grammarians who, following Bacon, postulated universal rules underlying all grammars. The concept of a universal grammar or language was at the core of the 17th century projects for philosophical languages.
Charles Darwin described language as an instinct in humans, like the upright posture. [ C.R. Darwin, "Descent of Man", John Murray, London, 1871.]

The idea rose to notability in modern linguistics with theorists such as Noam Chomsky and Richard Montague, developed in the 1950s to 1970s, as part of the "Linguistics Wars".

Chomsky's theory

Linguist Noam Chomsky made the argument that the human brain contains a limited set of rules for organizing language. In turn, there is an assumption that all languages have a common structural basis. This set of rules is known as "universal grammar".

Speakers proficient in a language know what expressions are acceptable in their language and what expressions are unacceptable. The key puzzle is how speakers should come to know the restrictions of their language, since expressions which violate those restrictions are not present in the input, indicated as such. This absence of negative evidence—that is, absence of evidence that an expression is part of a class of the ungrammatical sentences in one's language—is the core of the poverty of stimulus argument. For example, in English one cannot relate a question word like 'what' to a predicate within a relative clause (1):

(1) *What did John meet a man who sold?

Such expressions are not available to the language learners, because they are, by hypothesis, ungrammatical for speakers of the local language. Speakers of the local language do not utter such expressions and note that they are unacceptable to language learners. Universal grammar offers a solution to the poverty of the stimulus problem by making certain restrictions universal characteristics of human languages. Language learners are consequently never tempted to generalize in an illicit fashion.

Evidence and support

Neurological evidence

Recent evidence suggests part of the human brain (crucially involving Broca's area, a portion of the left inferior frontal gyrus), is selectively activated by those languages that meet Universal Grammar requirements.Musso, et al., [http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v6/n7/abs/nn1077.html Broca's area and the language instinct] ]

Presence of creole languages

The presence of creole languages is cited as further support for this theory, especially by Bickerton's controversial Language bioprogram theory. These languages were developed and formed when different societies came together and were forced to devise their own system of communication. The system used by the original speakers was an inconsistent mix of vocabulary items known as a pidgin. When these speakers' children were acquiring their first language, they used the pidgin input to effectively create their own original language, known as a creole. Unlike pidgins, creoles have native speakers and make use of a full grammar.

The idea of universal grammar is supported by the creole languages by virtue of the fact that certain features are shared by virtually all of these languages. For example, their default point of reference in time (expressed by bare verb stems) is not the present moment, but the past. Using pre-verbal auxiliaries, they uniformly express tense, aspect, and mood. Negative concord occurs, but it affects the verbal subject (as opposed to the object, as it does in languages like Spanish). Another similarity among creoles is that questions are created simply by changing a declarative sentence's intonation, not its word order or content.

Criticism

Some linguists oppose the universal grammar theory. Geoffrey Sampson maintains that universal grammar theories are not falsifiable and are therefore pseudo scientific theory, arguing that the grammatical generalizations made are simply observations about existing languages and not predictions about what is possible in a language.Fact|date=September 2008

Some feel that the basic assumptions of Universal Grammar are unfounded. Another way of defusing the poverty of the stimulus argument is if language learners notice the absence of classes of expressions in the input and, on this basis, hypothesize a restriction. This solution is closely related to Bayesian reasoning. Elman et al. argue that the unlearnability of languages assumed by UG is based on a too-strict, "worst-case" model of grammar.

James Hurford argues that the postulate of a "language acquisition device" essentially amounts to the trivial claim that languages are learnt by humans, and that the LAD isn't a theory so much as the "explanandum" looking for theories. [James R. Hurford, Nativist and Functional Explanations in Language Acquisition (1995), p. 88.]

The Pirahã language has been claimed by the linguist Daniel Everett to be a counterexample to Universal Grammar, showing properties allegedly unexpected under current views of Universal Grammar. Among other things, this language is alleged to lack all evidence for recursion, including embedded clauses, as well as quantifiers and color terms. [Everett, Daniel L. "Cultural Constraints on Grammar and Cognition in Pirahã: Another Look at the Design Features of Human Language." "Current Anthropology" 46.4 (2005): 621-646.] Some other linguists have argued, however, that some of these properties have been misanalyzed, and that others are actually expected under current theories of Universal Grammar. [Nevins, et al., PDFlink| [http://ling.auf.net/lingBuzz/ Pirahã Exceptionality: a Reassessment] .] While most languages studied in that respect do indeed seem to share common underlying rules, research is hampered by considerable sampling bias. Linguistically, most diverse areas such as tropical Africa and America, as well as the diversity of Indigenous Australian and Papuan languages, have been insufficiently studied. Furthermore, language extinction apparently has affected those areas most where most examples of unconventional languages have been found to dateFact|date=February 2007.

ee also

* Applicative Universal Grammar
* Broca's area
* Endolinguistics
* Language
* Native Language
* Origin of language
* Postmodernism in language
* Principles and parameters
* Psychological nativism
* Universal Networking Language

Notes

References

*Baker, Mark C. The Atoms of Language: The Mind's Hidden Rules of Grammar. Oxford University Press, 2003. ISBN 019860632X.
*Chomsky, N. "Aspects of the Theory of Syntax". MIT Press, 1965. ISBN 0-262-53007-4.
*Elman, J., Bates, E. et al. "Rethinking innateness." MIT Press, 1996.
*Sampson, G. "The "Language Instinct" Debate." Continuum International Publishing Group, 2005. ISBN 0826473849.
*Tomasello, M. "Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition." Harvard University Press, 2003. ISBN 0-674-01030-2.
*Window on Humanity. "A Concise Introduction to Anthropology". Conrad Phillip Kottak. Ed. Kevin Witt, Jill Gordon. The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. 2005
*White, Lydia. "Second Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar". Cambridge University Press, 2003. ISBN 0-521-79647-4
*Zuidema, Willem. "How the poverty of stimulus solves the poverty of stimulus." "Evolution of Language: Fourth International Conference", Harvard University, March 2002.


Wikimedia Foundation. 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать курсовую

Look at other dictionaries:

  • Universal grammar — U ni*ver sal gram mar, n. 1. (Linguistics) the principles forming the basis for the human ability to understand language. [PJC] 2. (Linguistics) the general properties and constraints common to all human languages; also, the study of such… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • universal grammar — noun : general grammar * * * Ling. 1. a grammar that attempts to establish the properties and constraints common to all possible human languages. 2. an innate system of principles underlying the human language faculty. * * * universal grammar UK… …   Useful english dictionary

  • universal grammar — Ling. 1. a grammar that attempts to establish the properties and constraints common to all possible human languages. 2. an innate system of principles underlying the human language faculty. * * * …   Universalium

  • universal grammar — See generative grammar …   Philosophy dictionary

  • universal grammar — u′niver′sal gram′mar n. 1) ling. a grammar that attempts to establish the properties and constraints common to all possible human languages 2) ling. the properties and constraints themselves • Etymology: 1930–35 …   From formal English to slang

  • universal grammar — noun Date: circa 1750 the study of general principles believed to underlie the grammatical phenomena of all languages; also such principles viewed as part of an innate human capacity for learning a language …   New Collegiate Dictionary

  • universal grammar — UK / US noun [uncountable] linguistics rules about language that apply to all languages …   English dictionary

  • Applicative Universal Grammar — Applicative Universal Grammar, or AUG, is a universal semantic metalanguage intended for studying the semantic processes in particular languages [ Shaumyan S. A Semiotic Theory of Language . Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press …   Wikipedia

  • Grammar — is the field of linguistics that covers the rules governing the use of any given natural language. It includes morphology and syntax, often complemented by phonetics, phonology, semantics, and pragmatics. Each language has its own distinct… …   Wikipedia

  • grammar — grammarless, adj. /gram euhr/, n. 1. the study of the way the sentences of a language are constructed; morphology and syntax. 2. these features or constructions themselves: English grammar. 3. an account of these features; a set of rules… …   Universalium

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”